The Inappropriately Excluded

Join us at MichaelWFerguson.substack.com

No person should be required to live under a body of laws, programs and policies that they consider to be fundamentally unjust. Politics is the art of compromise, but it is wrong to compromise one's principles. Today, there is no set of compromises that can result in a body of laws, programs and policies that do not violate a substantial portion of the population's sense of justness. Consequently, the Enlightened world must be comprised of several sovereign states with substantially different rules so that all can live within their concept of a just community. -Michael W Ferguson

The Inappropriately Excluded

The probability of entering and remaining in an intellectually elite profession such as Physician, Judge, Professor, Scientist, Corporate Executive, etc. increases with IQ to about 133.  It then falls about 1/3 by 140.  By 150 IQ the probability has fallen by 97%!  In other words, a significant percentage of people with IQs over 140 are being systematically and, most likely inappropriately, excluded from the population that addresses the biggest problems of our time or who are responsible for assuring the efficient operation of social, scientific, political and economic institutions.  This benefits neither the excluded group nor society in general. For society, it is a horrendous waste of a very valuable resource.  For the high IQ person it is a personal tragedy, commonly resulting in unrealized social, educational and productive potential.


The very limited research that has been done on this phenomenon has focused on possible flaws in high IQ people that might explain the exclusion.  In order to be explanatory, the flaw would need to increase  frequency with IQ.  However, the evidence that exists suggests that it is not the result of a compensatory flaw, but rather the result of inappropriate educational and productive environments within which the high IQ person must strive to succeed. Consequently, remediation should focus on creating more appropriate environments.

To this end, The Polymathic Institute promotes polymathic research, education, careers and lifestyles.  Polymathica is a global community of refinement and erudition.  The Institute membership will be comprised primarily of the inappropriately excluded population with an estimated ultimate membership potential of approximately one hundred thousand people.  Polymathica will have a larger population, perhaps as many as twelve to fifteen million.  It represents a proper social and career milieu for themselves, essentially the top 5% in intellectual sophistication, and for Institute members.

Neither Polymathica nor the Polymathic Institute are high IQ societies and no submission of IQ results is required.  Polymathicans and Institute Members generally have high IQs, however the organizational focus is on achievement, not potential.  We are interested in what you have done, what you are doing and what you plan on doing; we are not interested in what you could do, but won't.

The Exclusion
In the popular culture, IQ has become a point of contention.  Many people credulously accept that the eminent have very high IQs and that people of ordinary accomplishment have ordinary IQs.  For example, it was widely reported that Garry Kasparov has an IQ of 190.  In truth, his IQ is verified to be 135. Others, often based on Steven J. Gould's book, 'The Mismeasure of Man' subscribe to the assertion that IQ is a useless oversimplification that primarily measures how well a person takes IQ tests.

The science does not support either assertion.  A  very large body of scientific evidence shows that IQ tests measure a polygenetic trait, g, that exhibits moderate phenotypic variation.  It is directly correlated, over most of its range, with positive life outcomes and inversely correlated with negative ones.  It has also been shown to accurately measure what people mean when they use the words 'intelligent' or 'smart'.

However, because of the moderate r values of its correlates, IQ is primarily of value in understanding the characteristics and interactions of large populations.  Save as a diagnostic tool for very high IQ individuals, it is not sufficiently predictive to be reliably used on an individual case basis. Furthermore, in adulthood, actual life outcomes are generally known and, consequently, a predictive tool is of limited value.

When IQ tests first came out, the various intellectual elites were willing, even eager, to take them.  The results, however, while good, were not great, so today they generally are not so willing to have themselves tested.  Still, while most of the evidence is old, the results are still very likely to be valid. The only significant recent work is that of Robert Hauser and it suggests that, if anything, the mean IQ of the intellectually elite professions has fallen.  That, however, is almost surely an artifact of the methodology.


Over an extensive range of studies and with remarkable consistency, from Physicians to Professors to CEOs, the mean IQ of intellectually elite professions is about 125 and the standard deviationn is about 6.5.  For example, Gibson and Light found that 148 members of the Cambridge University faculty had a mean IQ of 126 with a standard deviation of 6.3.  The highest score was 139.  J.D. Matarazzo and S.G. Goldstein found that the mean IQ of 80 medical students was 125 with a standard deviation of about 6.7.  There was one outlier at 149, but the next highest score was 138.  This means that 95% of people in intellectually elite professions have IQs between 112 and 138   99.98% have IQs between 99 and 151.

By dividing the distribution function of the elite professions' IQ by that of the general population, we can calculate the relative probability that a person of any given IQ will enter and remain in an intellectually elite profession.  We find that the probability increases to about 133 and then begins to fall.  By 140 it has fallen by about 1/3 and by 150 it has fallen by about 97%.  In other words, for some reason, the 140s are really tough on one's prospects for joining an intellectually elite profession.  It seems that people with IQs over 140 are being systematically, and likely inappropriately, excluded.  With the conservative assumption that, absent the exclusionary processes, IQs above 133 neither help nor hinder the achievement of elite profession membership, the excluded population is distributed as shown above.  If we assume that the positive correlation seen below 133 IQ continues above 133, the excluded population would be larger and the exclusion more complete.

Grady Towers, in his article, 'The Empty Promise' concludes that IQs over 140 add nothing to the academic or career performance of the individual.  However, the result herein described is a stronger statement in that it actually appears to support an inverse correlation.  It is not an entirely new revelation.  Robert Sternberg and others have mentioned an inverse correlation by observing the absence of very high IQ individuals in intellectual settings.  However, the observation has not led to any deep investigation.  Typically, it is mentioned with an implication that very high IQ people routinely possess some compensating negative trait that eliminates their intellectual advantage.  An example is the assertion that very high IQ people lack 'common sense'. Dressed up, this is Sternberg's hypothesis.  Another explanation is that decreasing 'emotional intelligence' nullifies the advantage of higher IQ. Little research has actually been done on the exclusion and what little that has does not support either of these explanations.  Linda Gottfredson has argued energentically against the Sternberg model.

While increasing IQ, especially over 140, is inversely correlated with elite membership, 140-150 IQ is also characteristic of eminence (Nobelists, Fields Medalists, etc.).  While there are a number of anecdotal and inferential citations, the most definitive study was that of Dr. Anne Roe (1952) in which she gave 64 of America's (U.S. born) most eminent scientists an IQ test that ETS had created for that purpose.  As best as can be determined (there were methodological problems) the test rendered a 15 point ratio IQ and the average IQ of the group was 152.  This corresponds to a modern deviation IQ of 144 which agrees with the anecdotal and inferential evidence.

                         Low.      Median   High
Verbal              121            166       177
Spatial              123            137      164
Math                128            154    194
Averages          124            152       178

What this suggests is that while an IQ over 140 will decrease the probability of entrance into an elite profession, if the impediment can be overcome, performance within the elite is likely to be superior. Of the 64, the highest D15IQ was 158, which is close to the statistically expected highest IQ of any scientist.  In other words, by 160 D15IQ, the exclusion is nearly complete and by this study of the most eminent, the statistical prediction is corroborated.  However, in total, this higher IQ characteristic of eminence strongly supports the conclusion that the exclusion is inappropriate and if these extremely high IQ individuals were allowed to work on the hardest problems, the result would be eminence.

As will be discussed later, Mathematics and Theoretical Physics contain many problems that are difficult to solve but relatively easy to verify.  As such, they may be exceptions to this exclusion.  The reason that the Roe study doesn't reflect this is because the Physicists and Mathematicians were not given the math portion which we can assume lowered their average score.

So, if your IQ is 140 something, the above should serve as a warning that you may be facing related career challenges.  If your IQ is over 150, it is a clarion call; without direct intervention, your career prospects are very poor.  If you are the parent of a child with a D15IQ over 150, immediate and dramatic action is required.  At present, realistic options for individual remediation are severely limited.

To provide perspective for readers, one in 261 people have IQs over 140 and one in 2,331 have IQs over 150. While the high IQ exclusion does not directly affect a large percentage of the population, the people it does affect, it affects profoundly.  Because of the large population of western civiliztion, the absolute number in this group is not small.  There are approximately 6.5 million people with an IQ over 140 and 729,000 people with an IQ over 150.

Follow me at MichaelWFerguson.Substack.com.

Why is This Happening?
Because of the dearth of objective evidence, the cause of the exclusion cannot be determined directly.  Garth Zietsman has said, referring to people with D15IQs over 152, 'A common experience with people in this category or higher is that they are not wanted - the masses (including the professional classes) find them an affront of some sort.'  While true, it is more likely a symptom than a cause of the exclusion.  We need to understand why they are an affront.

From a theoretical standpoint, democratic meritocracies should evolve five IQ defined 'castes', The Leaders, The Advisors, The Followers, The Clueless and The Excluded. These castes are natural in that they are the result of how people of different intellectual abilities relate to one another.  This is based on research done by Leta Hollingworth in the 1930's and the more recent work of D.K. Simonton.

Before we begin, we need to digress for a moment into a discussion of deviation and ratio IQs. Because few people understand the difference, there has been significant confusion over the meaning of various IQ scores.  IQ was originally designed for children and was defined as ((mental age)/(chronological age))X100.  In other words, an eight year old with a 150 IQ scored about the same as the average twelve year old.

It was found very quickly that there were far more very high IQ children than what the standard, Gaussian distribution predicts.  So, today, IQ tests have their raw scores adjusted to force the results to fit a standard bell curve distribution and are referred to as deviation IQs. While this practice has benefits, it tends to depress the IQs of the very highest scorers and, thereby, understate the intellectual distance between them and more normal IQs.  For example, a person with a 170 IQ today would have a 200 IQ in the ratio IQ era.

Leta Hollingworth studied profoundly gifted children.  She reported them as having IQs of 180+, which was a R16 score.  As such, on today's tests this equates to 159+.  Her conclusion was that when IQ differences are greater than 30 points, leader/follower relationships will break down or will not form.  It establishes an absolute limit to the intellectual gulf between leader and followers.  She also concluded that there was an D15IQ 'sweet spot' of best outcomes from 123 to 144.

We have no reason to conclude that this upper limit on IQ differences changes in adulthood and, consequently, an elite with a mean R16IQ of 128 will have no leaders with R16IQs over 158 (149 D15IQ).  This is consistent with the conclusion that there are no appropriate roles for >150 D15IQs and approximately corroborates Hollingworth's 'sweet spot'.

Much more recently, D.K. Simonton found that persuasiveness is at its maximum when the IQ differential between speaker and audience is about 20 points.  While he has not studied this effect among those with very high IQs, it is assumed that it follows ratio IQs at the high end.  This has been corroborated with empirical studies of manager and leader success, which peaks between a 1.0 and 1.2 standard deviation differential.

We are going to use ratio IQs to perform our calculations, as they are probably a more accurate measure of intellectual distance at the high end.  However, for clarity, we will restate our answers to the modern standard of 15 point deviation IQs.

We already know that elites have an average IQ of about 125 (R16 128) which implies that the audience that is to be convinced by the elites has a mean R16IQ of 108 (D15IQ is about the same under 120 IQ).  People with R16IQs below 98, after Hollingworth, are not effective followers and in a modern meritocracy are essentially disenfranchised and in the public discourse, essentially 'The Clueless'.  It means that the 'The Followers' in the public discourse have a R16IQ mode of 108 R16IQ and 'The Leaders' have a R16IQ mode of 128 (125 D15IQ).  These calculations provide us with a theoretical understanding of why the intellectually elite professions so consistently have mean D15IQs of 125.

In free markets people choose to whom they listen.  In other words, in audiences dominated by high school graduates, who average around 105 IQ, the successful leaders will have an average IQ of 105+20=125.  Speakers with R16IQs over 105+30=135 (D15IQ130) will be cancelled from radio, fired from TV and print or not elected because they confuse rather than enlighten their audience.  A college educated audience (115 IQ) will be most convinced by a R16IQ of 115+20=135 and confused by a 115+30=145 R16IQ (140 D15IQ).

Effective leaders recognize that they need the counsel of those smarter than themselves.  They will be most convinced by advisors with R16IQs of 128+20=148 (D15IQ 139).  We also see that the compressed standard deviation is predicted as a result of persuasive needs of the overall organizational structure.  A Leader needs to be persuasive within the community of Leaders which limits the R16IQ to 128+20=148 which is the same as the mode for Advisors.  However, the 148 R16IQ Leader becomes incomprehensible to most Followers, which limits their effectiveness and encourages them to become an Advisor. Because Leaders become ineffective above an R16IQ of 148, Advisors won't find clients if their R16IQ is over 148+20=168=155 D15IQ.

So we see that these parameters of maximum persuasiveness of 20 R16 points and maximum leader/follower differential of 30 R16 points, create a natural trifurcation of enfranchised people into 'The Advisors' (128-168 R16IQ; 125-155 D15IQ), Leaders (115-141 R16IQ; 112-138 D15IQ) and Followers (98-128 R16IQ; 98-125 D15IQ)  'The Clueless' with D15 IQs below 98 are effectively lost to the process.  They cannot really understand the public discourse and will often not follow discussions in productive environments.

People with D15IQs over 150 are effectively 'The Excluded', routinely finding their thoughts to be unconvincing in the public discourse and in productive environments.  If placed in a leadership position, they will not succeed.

So, while Sternberg et alia search for personal flaws to explain professional and social failings for people with D15IQs>150, the simple fact is that it is an artifact of a culture that fails to provide them with audience or followers.  They are not a natural fit as advisors because the leaders are not persuaded and often won't even understand the advice.

Inappropriate Educational Options
The exclusion really begins in primary school with the failure of the educational process to provide an appropriate learning environment.  The grading process, which should be a reliable assessment of knowledge learned and skills acquired, becomes nothing more than a measure of the child's willingness to bend to the will of the teachers' demand that he or she acquiesce to a profoundly inappropriate curriculum and learning process.

Leta Hollingworth noted that, if mainstreamed, children with R16IQs over 150 (D15IQ 141) check out and do not excel.  Miraca Gross has done a long-term longitudinal study of 60, 160+ D15IQ Australian children. 17 of the children were radically accelerated, 10 were accelerated one or two years and the remaining 33 were mainstreamed.  The results were astonishing with every radically accelerated student reported as educationally and professionally successful and emotionally and socially satisfied.  The group that was not accelerated she characterizes as follows: 'With few exceptions, they have very jaded views of their education. Two dropped out of high school and a number have dropped out of university. Several more have had ongoing difficulties at university, not because of a lack of ability but because they have found it difficult to commit to undergraduate study that is less than stimulating'. These children have IQs similar to Leonardo da Vinci, Galileo, etc., so the loss from unrealized potential is enormous.

Gross also did a wonderful comparative case study of a 133 D15IQ girl who had great educational success and a 169 D15IQ boy who was completely destroyed by an uncaring school system.  It provides some enlightening examples of precisely how the educational system thwarts children in the 140+ D15IQ range.

The problem stems from the misconception among educators that the intellectual gulf between moderately and highly gifted children is not that great.  In fact, depending upon the conceptual content, Professor Gross suggests that the exceptionally gifted children and above may learn 4-5 times faster than the midrange students.  Therefore, a reasonable, in fact conservative, expectation of educational progress is the ratio of the highly gifted student's ratio IQ and the ratio IQ for which the curriculum is normed.

So, a 150 D15IQ child would be expected to progress through a K-12 public school curriculum geared to the 100 IQ student in 12/1.6=7.5 years.  They would graduate from high school at 13. Some children may be physically and emotionally prepared for full time school a year early and would finish high school at 12. When we hear about a child who finishes high school at 12 or 13, we think of a 'one in a million' prodigy and we suspect that the child was pushed to his or her detriment.  Yet, with an enabling educational environment, it is actually a reasonable expectation for about one in 200 children.  The true 'one in a million' child is doing college level learning at 7 or 8.

These children can be expected to complete their six years of college, which is geared to a 120 IQ, in about 6/(160/120)=4.5 years.  So, we would expect the 150 D15IQ person to receive their first advanced degree at age 17 or 18 if the educational system didn't actively retard them. This will provide them with another five or six years of education, during which they can acquire another four advanced degrees or equivalent.

It is often stated that gifted children become bored in mainstream classes. However, that is too passive a description.  Often they are frustrated and even angered by the slow pace. Garth Zietsman states that people with IQs over 124 'don't require assistance to learn. They can find the information and master the methods themselves'. It is probably the case that for most 140+ D15IQ people, autodidactic or self paced learning is preferred.  It is also likely that they prefer the polymathic 'question first' approach to learning, as well.

Because of all the above, many, perhaps most, 150+ D15IQ children reach college age with a bad grade transcript and even worse attitude.  Even if they manage to perform near their potential in their educational careers, it will likely not matter since adult society is not structured for them, anywa

Social Isolation
What applies to productive environments also applies to social environments and even personal relationships.  Theoretically, after Hollingworth, a person's social relationships should be limited to people with R16IQs within 30 points of their own.  For the 100 IQ person, this will include about 94% of the population and consequently it is not an issue.  However, for the 150 R16IQ (140 D15IQ), social relationships are limited to 120-180 R16IQ people which represents just a little over 10% of the population.  The 165 R16IQ (150D15IQ) person will be limited to people with 135+ R16IQs (130 D15IQ).  This comprises just 2% of the population.   By 182 R16IQ (160 D15IQ) the problem becomes critical with social relationships limited to those with R16IQs over 152 (142 D15IQ) which comprises just 0.25% of the population.

The +/- 30 R16IQ range of Leta Hollingworth is also a good estimated limit on lasting social relationships.  However, they are not equal relationships but rather will necessarily have a strong leader/follower quality to them.  Also, the degree of mutual understanding will almost surely be insuffient to reach and sustain emotional intimacy.  Relationships based upon approximate intellectual parity probably cannot have more than 0.75 standard deviation (~12 points).  For the 140 D15IQ person, the limit for intellectual parity relationships is about 128, or about 2.5% of the population.  For the Hollingsworth children, 180 R16IQ (159 D15IQ), the limit for an intellectual parity relationship is a hopeless 168+ R16IQ or 152+ D15IQ.  This is only 0.0263% of the population.

Members of high IQ societies, especially those that require D15IQs above 145, often comment that around this IQ, qualitatively different thinking emerges.  By this they mean that the 145+ D15IQ person doesn't just do the same things, intellectually, as a lower IQ person, just faster and more accurately, but actually engages in fundamentally different intellectual processes.  David Wechsler, D. K. Simonton, et alia, have observed the same thing.

Since intimate social relationships are predicated upon mutual understanding, this draws a kind of 'line in the sand' at 140-150 D15IQ that appears to separate humans into two distinct groups.  This may truncate the 30 point limit for those between 150 and 160 D15IQ people. Even when 150+ D15IQ people learn to function in the mainstream society, they will always be considered, and will feel, in some way 'different'.  Grady Towers explored this in depth in his article, 'The Outsiders'.  This is of mild interest to the group within which the 150+ D15IQ person is embedded but it is moderately to profoundly important to the high IQ individual who will feel an often profound sense of isolation.

It has often been observed that 150+ D15IQ people are loners.  Also, Loius Termann found that children at this IQ level were emotionally maladjusted in about 40% of the cases.  However from the above one cannot help but wonder if this results from the children being constantly thrust into 'no-win' social situations and never given the opportunity to hone their social skills among their intellectual peers.

Assortative mating, in humans includes a strong tendency to choose a spouse who is in the same IQ range.  At a maximum, IQ difference cannot exceed Hollingworth's 30 points and preferentially should be within Simonton's 20 points.  For the 100 IQ person 80-120 IQ contains about 80% of the population and not much thought about intelligence is neccesary when choosing a mate.  However, the 150 D15IQ percent will find that less than 0.4% of prospective mates are in the proper intellectual range.  Because of this, IQ becomes a significant limitation on mate selection.

These factors probably explain the positive correlation between higher IQ and emotional maladjustment found by Terman, et alia.  It is not an inherent trait of high intelligence but rather a consequence of extreme social isolation.

Conclusion
As D15IQ increases above 140, people become progressively more excluded from educational, productive and social opportunities until by 160 D15IQ the exclusion is nearly complete.

Individuals with D15IQs of more than 160 are rare, comprising just 0.0032% of the population. They possess at least one trait in common with many of the greatest minds in history.  Yet only a vanishingly small percentage will find a proper environment within which they may thrive intellectually, socially and productively.

This is harmful for the individuals but it is also an unfortunate circumstance for society as well. What if intellectual giants like Einstein, da Vinci, J.S. Mill, etc. were ten times more common? Almost certainly progress would be much greater.  It is because of this that the exclusion should be of significant concern to everyone.

The Polymathic Institute and Polymathica
Many people with D15IQs between 140 and 150 and nearly all people with IQs over 150 face enormous challenges and require new social and productive environments if they are to reach their potential.  The Polymathic Institute promotes polymathic research, education, careers and lifestyles

Polymathica is targeted at the upper 5% of the population in intellectual sophistication.  This is approximately equivalent to D15IQs above 125.  Leaders will, characteristically, have R16IQs over 148 (D15IQ 147) and up to 178 R16IQ (166 D15IQ).  Advisors will have D15IQs of 161+.

Clearly, few of the people of working age with D15IQs over 150 have appropriate outlets.  The only probable exceptions are Mathematics and Theoretical Physics where the range of comprehensibility is probably closer to 60 R16IQ points than to the 30 R16IQ point Hollingsworth limit.  However, the careers are only appropriate, with regard to interest and disposition, for a small percentage of the 150+ D15IQ population.  The vast majority have no appropriate career options.

Over time, The Polymathic Institute may attract as much as 25% of those with a 150+ D15IQ. If they comprise about 1% of Polymathica, Polymathica will reach about 12 million in membership. That is about 15% of the top 5% and consistent with current evidence.

In other words, we can, if we succeed, enable a significant portion of those currently inapropriately excluded from participating in the hardest problems and the most intellectually demanding projects.  



Join Polymathica

Those who are interested in participating in either Polymathica or The Polymathic Institute should subscribe to the Institute's newsletter, The Polymath.  To do so, just provide an e-mail address, name (optional) and referral code.  If you were not referred, enter 999999.

Bibliography
A Nation Deceived How Schools Hold Back America’s Brightest Students
Edited by Nicholas Colangelo, Susan G. Assouline and Miraca U. M. Gross
https://www.accelerationinstitute.org/Nation_Deceived/ND_v2.pdf:

Understanding and Being Understood The Impact of Intelligence and Dispositional Valuations on Social Relationships
Jacobus J. A. Denissen,
http://edoc.hu-berlin.de/dissertationen/denissen-jacobus-josephus-adrianus-2005-07-08/PDF/Denissen.pdf

Intelligence Among University Students
Gibson, J., and P. Light
Nature 213: 441–443.

Exceptionally Gifted Children
Miraca UM Gross
http://www.amazon.com/Exceptionally-Gifted-Children-Miraca-Gross/dp/0415314917

Exceptionally and Profoundly Gifted Students:
An Underserved Population
Miraca UM Gross
http://www.hoagiesgifted.org/underserved.htm

Exceptionally Gifted Children:  Long-Term Outcomes of Academic Acceleration and Nonacceleration
Miraca U. M. Gross
http://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/EJ746290.pdf

Perceptions of Causes and Long Term Effects of Academic Underachievement in High IQ Adults

Favier-Townsend, Anne Madeleine MarieDate: 2015-10-12http://hdl.handle.net/2299/16520

Attachment Styles and Experience of Workplace Interpersonal Relating in Intellectually Gifted Adults.
Sonja Esterhuyse Falck
 
6 December 2013
https://ihbv.nl/cms/wp-content/uploads/2015/01/Sonja-Esterhuyse-Falck-Research-Report.pdf
The intellectual caliber of medical students
J.D. Matarazzo, S.G. Goldstein
Journal of Medical Education, Volume 47, Issue 2, 1972, pp. 102–111

Meritocracy, cognitive ability, and the sources of occupational success
Robert M. Hauser
https://www.ssc.wisc.edu/cde/cdewp/98-07.pdf

Children Above 180 IQ Standford-Binet
Leta Stetter Hollingsworth
http://www.gutenberg.org/ebooks/47403

The Making of a Scientist
Roe, Anne
http://www.amazon.com/The-Making-Scientist-Anne-Roe/dp/0837171512

Intelligence and personal influence in groups: Four nonlinear models.
Simonton, D. K. (1985).
Psychological Review, 92, 532-547.

The Outsiders
Towers, Grady
http://prometheussociety.org/cms/articles/the-outsiders

The Empty Promise
Towers, Grady
http://miyaguchi.4sigma.org/gradytowers/emptypromise.html

35 comments:

  1. Hi, thanks for the interesting article.

    You say that "By dividing the distribution function of the elite professions' IQ by that of the general population, we can calculate the relative probability that a person of any given IQ will enter and remain in an intellectually elite profession. We find that the probability increases to about 133 and then begins to fall."

    Could you show the precise math?
    Having CLD I really love math. Thank you
    G. D.

    ReplyDelete
    Replies
    1. It is simply the gaussian distribution for median 126, SD 6.5 divided by the gaussian distribution for median 100, SD 15. This format just isn't going to allow me to write out the formula, but it is available at https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Normal_distribution

      Thanks for the kind words.

      Michael

      Delete
  2. You can find The Empty Promise at http://miyaguchi.4sigma.org/gradytowers/emptypromise.html

    ReplyDelete
  3. "Polymath" is a good word. I hope "Melvin" (Kevin L., the bane of the high-IQ societies, who has a personal website with "polymath" in the name) doesn't hassle you for the use of the word the way he stole the Mega Society from Chris Langan with a default judgement.

    Though I'm a high IQ social dropout, I'm not persuaded that those with high IQs are grossly underrepresented in academia. These links seem to suggest otherwise. (From physics prof. Steve Hsu's blog, Information Processing, using SMPY data)

    Since you quoted GradyTowers, you may be interested in the Rasch or absolute intelligence scale, which allows people of different ages and standard deviation scores to be compared. See slide 19 of this presentation by Dr. McGrew, who works on the Woodcock-Johnson IQ test which, though it is just for the block rotation subtest, should be pretty close to the full-scale IQ chart. I have spent many hours looking at this chart in paint.net, which allows measuring distances using the line tool. +3 s.d. 5 year olds have intelligence similar to the average college graduate, about 0.5 s.d. above the adult mean. By age 12, they're as smart as an adult with a 120 IQ (s.d.15).

    Adults 1 s.d. below average have intelligence similar to an average 10 year old, adults 2 s.d. below average (not quite retarded) are about at the level of an average 6 year old (and don't get more intelligent after age 10). By age 4.5, children above +2 s.d. are smarter than -1 s.d. adults. It's clear that today's system disenfranchises and infantilizes gifted children who are objectively more intellectually capable than many or even most adults.

    ReplyDelete
    Replies
    1. This comment has been removed by the author.

      Delete
    2. Kevin and I know each other and Ron and I have met and discussed his tests. Kevin and Ron have had disagreements and I don't take sides. They're both winding down anyway.

      Steve Hsu and I have discussed this and he's mostly wrong. The discussion is complex. A couple of brief points. By dividing the distribution of intellectual elites by the distribution of the general population, a theoretical distribution of relative inclusion is derived. It hardly compels every person with an IQ over 133. And that tail could be and probably is fat. Also, I believe math and theoretical physics are likely less excluded because they tend to involve problems that are much harder to solve than to verify. Hollingsworth's limit may not apply or may be more than 30 points. However, Robert Hauser still found data that implied an exclusion in those fields. And the WAIS scores of Cambridge faculty doesn't support it.

      Steve relied on the study of mathematically precocious and the follow up in adulthood. Somewhere here I wrote a rebuttal. It's complicated, but basically by using their SAT-V I estimated the IQs to be in a range that is much lower than he argued. Plus most of the inclusion as adults arose from using their mathematical talent, not their IQ.

      Anyway, thanks for the input.

      Delete
  4. "Polymath" is a good word. I hope "Melvin" (Kevin L., the bane of the high-IQ societies, who has a personal website with "polymath" in the name) doesn't hassle you for the use of the word the way he stole the Mega Society from Chris Langan with a default judgement.

    Though I'm a high IQ social dropout, I'm not persuaded that those with high IQs are grossly underrepresented in academia. These links seem to suggest otherwise. (From physics prof. Steve Hsu's blog, Information Processing, using SMPY data)

    Since you quoted GradyTowers, you may be interested in the Rasch or absolute intelligence scale, which allows people of different ages and standard deviation scores to be compared. See slide 19 of this presentation by Dr. McGrew, who works on the Woodcock-Johnson IQ test which, though it is just for the block rotation subtest, should be pretty close to the full-scale IQ chart. I have spent many hours looking at this chart in paint.net, which allows measuring distances using the line tool. +3 s.d. 5 year olds have intelligence similar to the average college graduate, about 0.5 s.d. above the adult mean. By age 12, they're as smart as an adult with a 120 IQ (s.d.15).

    Adults 1 s.d. below average have intelligence similar to an average 10 year old, adults 2 s.d. below average (not quite retarded) are about at the level of an average 6 year old (and don't get more intelligent after age 10). By age 4.5, children above +2 s.d. are smarter than -1 s.d. adults. It's clear that today's system disenfranchises and infantilizes gifted children who are objectively more intellectually capable than many or even most adults.

    ReplyDelete
  5. A very interesting topic for discussion at TEDx Miami. Sign me up.

    ReplyDelete
  6. A very interesting topic for discussion at TEDx Miami. Sign me up.

    ReplyDelete
    Replies
    1. As I expected, they ignored me and this article. As Garth Zietsman says, we are an affront of some sort.

      Delete
  7. just a quick look, and quoting
    "Polymathica is a global community of refinemmemt and erudition."
    There's a typo above , in 'refinement'; for the rest not such a bad article although the author seems to be the sort of person who thinks he's always right(eg with respect to Russia). The Russians shot down MH17 with a Buck its a fact. greetings to you Russian wife, in Russian there's no free press, and they are brainwashed by propaganda.

    ReplyDelete
  8. and another:
    "Because of the large population of western civiliztion"
    typo in 'civilization' NB if your IQ is so high mr Ferguson, maybe next time use a spelling checker when writing such articles (i don't blame you if you're slightly dyslectic; in fact i only see it now after a few beers, normally i read fast and overlook such tiny mistakes; good luck and f*ck the kgb fsb svr gru and VVP

    ReplyDelete
    Replies
    1. Whew. Still good with the CIA? Remember, the NSA is the only government agency that actually listens to you.

      Delete
  9. This is so interesting. So, basically what you're saying is that you're so smart that you cannot figure out how to speak to people who are less intelligent than you, but yet, people who are less intelligent than you can still figure out how to speak to you. Truly an incredible paradox.

    ReplyDelete
    Replies
    1. No, that is a misunderstanding. Of course, despite my IQ I can speak comprehensively to a two year old. But no matter how I try, I cannot get a two year old to understand the categorical imperative. The concept is simply beyond him. If I want to explain an idea that is at the 115 IQ level to someone with a 125 I, I can be successful. But if I want to explain a 155 IQ idea to that someone, I will fail. Not because there is something wrong with my explaining ability, but because the idea is beyond them. Simonton found that 20IQ points is best. Hollingsworth found that over 30 starts to exceed the ability of the listener. Without testing it, this is what Simonton gave as the explanation for the fall off over 20 points.

      Delete
    2. Good explanation, but also only states part of the problem. As mentioned in the article, highly intelligent people don't just perform the same functions faster and more accurately, they perform qualitatively different intellectual processes. There are 'intuitive' leaps that can be hard to recognize by the gifted as they seem simply obvious or inevitable consequence of the problem. Those in a totally different IQ range are unable to follow, become confused, lose interest or disengage from the subject.

      This can be overcome with clear, simple communication but that fact can be non-obvious to the gifted mind. Intellectual capacity and communication are linked, but I would argue not always strongly. Communication is a learned skill based on aptitude, not an inherent trait. True brilliance is often difficult to follow. I scull about in the shallow end of the 'gifted' pool at around D15IQ 138. While I follow many intellectual areas, I often find myself fighting to follow the truly brilliant work of some. They're not bad communicators, they're just on a different level from me.

      Thank you for the article. Somehow, I've never come across Hollingsworth's work before. It may take me a while to properly sort through and understand, but it does seem to explain so much about leadership, intellect and social outcomes.

      Delete
  10. It's a very interesting article. Also I have a question : is the scale used Cattell or Wechsler ?

    ReplyDelete
  11. This comment has been removed by the author.

    ReplyDelete
  12. Because if the scale Cattell is used, the mean IQ of intellectually elite professions is not 125 but roughly 116

    ReplyDelete
    Replies
    1. I designate D15IQ, D16IQ and R160IQ. Cattell is a D24 scale and one would assume that the elites would score 142. Interesting story. Mensa was originally meant to be a 3σ society. But they used the Cattell thinking it was a D16 scale. When they discovered the mistake they had to choose between kicking out most of their members or changing it to a 2σ society. They chose the latter.

      Both the WAIS and the SB are D15 scales now. However, the WAIS is strictly Gaussian and above about 140 SB tail is fat. No explanation from SB.

      Delete
  13. This comment has been removed by the author.

    ReplyDelete
  14. The 133IQ group are just smart but not smart enough to realize the long-term effects of their administrating their administrative duties to up their paychecks at the cost of smothering the really smart people and wasting the treasuries of the nations they run. It's the administrators. Smart enough to fool their followers and cast aspersions on true geniuses.

    ReplyDelete
    Replies
    1. Wow that was an amazing comment.

      Delete
    2. escapedmentalpatientJune 9, 2023 at 9:36 PM

      How do we know that the administrators know what they are doing? Personnel decisions are often made by people "following their gut" without actually understanding why their "instincts" are shying them away from one candidate and toward another.

      Delete
  15. Can you send me the updated link to join Polymathica? The link in the article is not functioning. Thank you.

    ReplyDelete
  16. I have not gotten formally tested, but in all that I have read, I likely fall 130 - 150. This is an incredibly insightful article, and every experience of my 28 years of life proves these theories to be accurate

    ReplyDelete
  17. What an awesome post, I just read it from start to end your blog post, an informative blog.
    https://blog.mindvalley.com/lowest-iq-ever/

    ReplyDelete
  18. Very interesting article. It was one of the search results related to Grady Towers' Outsiders. I can affirm their validity, at least anecdotally, as I can relate to them. They have helped me gain insight into myself. It is a difficult challenge, which society is largely failing, to offer extreme IQ individuals the intellectual stimulation to fulfill their potential.

    While most of my primary education was in private schools, I was not advanced beyond the normal grade level for my age. I did score 12.9 on the first EA test I was given at the beginning of my 6th grade year. Being constantly bored in school resulted in repeatedly skipping class and many lost years of potential learning opportunities. It was a pattern I carried with me into college. Since my grades did not suffer, no one cared what I did.

    My best teacher, whom I am most thankful for, taught 5th grade math. She gave me her son's used college calculus book. That was the only time I was offered the chance to enjoy the challenge of learning. Without motivational challenges in our lives we will be unable to discover our true potentials.

    ReplyDelete
  19. The reality of even the most lauded of professions is that much of it is fairly routine drudgery. I cannot emphasise enough how harmful intellectual overload at undergraduate level is to those then told to spend 4-5 years doing menial practical manipulations in laboratories as PhD students. The intellectual requirements of that is almost zero, yet the educational preparation for it almost entirely irrelevant. You see a higher level of depression and even suicide amongst fizzing young minds thrown to the wolves in neglecting laboratory environments. It was always covered up by academia, they found ways to rationalise, move people on. Not once in eleven years did I ever see any academic institution take ownership of the shameful things they had done to young people....

    Either you can get practical things to work easily or you can't. The number of things that could be wrong is sufficiently large that 'reinventing the wheel' is simply not acceptable. But that is what many PhD students were told to do.

    Go look at the intellectually gifted but practically scatterbrained as the key population.

    They are the most discriminated group in most western cultures. Bullied at school, contempt oozed for them by plenty of academics.

    So the sane ones go do something else, like work in a ski resort. For non-graduates with empathy.

    Don't try and change institutions that can't be changed.

    Just change HR policies and careers advice to the ones who shouldn't go anywhere near academia.

    ReplyDelete
  20. Don't you think your going too far with your conclusions that are based only on a few tiny studies that aren't necessarily methodologically rigorous. I can elaborate much more if you'd like.

    ReplyDelete
  21. it's how people conceive of free-time. the 120's perceive that it's 'time to do something', where the 140's will hunt for more waves to surf - of novel discovery without assigning feeling like they should have been getting 'something' done.

    ReplyDelete
  22. I think that the only way to improve the situation is to speak publicly on the subject (I believe there're many highly intelligent individuals who have social influence) or connect all the people who have a similar problem and demonstrate, propose concrete solutions and reliefs and explain why it is important for them personally and also for the rest of the world. The same goes for other neurodivergent folks. It is ingrained in human instinct to recognize the different as dangerous and to avoid it. I began to expect less from people and to rationalize their behaviors and thoughts by previously studying their way of thinking and processing data. There are ways to bypass their activation of instincts (I mean in the way information is transmitted). Frustrations are inevitable and frequent, though and one should learn to accept them and never give up. Thank you for such great insights.

    ReplyDelete
  23. Michael, your post is very enlightening. To the least, it converges with my own thoughts on IQ and social exclusion since I was a teen (we actually discussed this, albeit briefly, back then - a decade ago). Besides feeling a sense of restored hope to see that this issues are being addressed by others and not just "the monsters in my mental closet" I want to say that perhaps the reason for this consistent exclusion can be (mildly) explained by the dunbars number. Humans have the common limite to keep connections with circa 150 people throught their lives, a rarity that equates to an IQ of about 138 sd15 (this being the limit for the masses to "get along with"). The exclusion goes into further parcels regarding that statistical ceilling should be about 195 sd15.
    I guess the question is, "why does the Dunbar number exists as a built paradigm?" "Is it societal ir a trully anthropological end?" Im quite hoping for it not to be the latter. Not ironically, I write this thread alone, at a disco club night. I'll speak more on this subject soon. Thank you Michael, best wishes.

    ReplyDelete